Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.

Yıl: 2020 Cilt: 31 Sayı: 2 Sayfa Aralığı: 323 - 337 Metin Dili: İngilizce DOI: http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214 İndeks Tarihi: 15-09-2021

Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.

Öz:
-
Anahtar Kelime:

Belge Türü: Makale Makale Türü: Kitap Tanıtımı Erişim Türü: Bibliyografik
  • Bemis, D.K. ve Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magnetoencephalography investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801-2814.
  • Bemis, D.K. ve Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, 23(8), 1859-1873.
  • Bögels, S., Schriefers, H., Vonk, W., Chwilla, D.J. ve Kerkhofs, R. (2010). The interplay between prosody and syntax in sentence processing: The case of subject-and object-control verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1036-1053.
  • Campbell, T., Winkler, I. ve Kujala, T. (2007). N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 530–40.
  • Capek, C.M., Grossi, G., Newman, A.J., McBurney, S.L., Corina, D., Roeder, B. ve Neville, H.J. (2009). Brain systems mediating semantic and syntactic processing in deaf native signers: Biological invariance and modality specificity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8784–8789.
  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dupoux, E. ve Gout, A. (2000). Electrophysiological correlates of phonological processing: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 635-647.
  • Eckstein, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (2005). Late interaction of syntactic and prosodic processes in sentence comprehension as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 130-143.
  • Eckstein, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (2006). It's early: event-related potential evidence for initial interaction of syntax and prosody in speech comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(10), 1696-1711.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78-84.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357-1392.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(5), 262-268.
  • Friederici, A.D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267-276.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Frisch, S. (2000). Verb-argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 476–507.
  • Friederici, A.D. ve Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax–semantic interface. Brain research, 1146, 50-58.
  • Friederici, A.D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R.I. ve Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(7), 2458-2463.
  • Friederici, A.D., Fiebach, C.J., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I.D. ve von Cramon, D.Y. (2006). Processing linguistic complexity and grammaticality in the left frontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1709-1717.
  • Friederici, A.D., Friedrich, M. ve Weber, C. (2002). Neural manifestation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early infancy. Neuroreport, 13(10), 1251-1254.
  • Friederici, A.D., Meyer, M. ve von Cramon, D.Y. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and Language, 74(2), 289-300.
  • Friederici, A.D., Rüschemeyer, S.A., Hahne, A. ve Fiebach, C.J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13(2), 170-177.
  • Goucha, T. ve Friederici, A.D. (2015). The language skeleton after dissecting meaning: a functional segregation within Broca's Area. Neuroimage, 114, 294-302.
  • Grewe, T., Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Wiese, R., von Cramon, D.Y. ve Schlesewsky, M. (2005). The emergence of the unmarked: A new perspective on the language‐specific function of Broca's area. Human Brain Mapping, 26(3), 178-190.
  • Gunter, T.C., Friederici, A.D. ve Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556-568.
  • Gunter, T.C., Friederici, A.D. ve Hahne, A. (1999). Brain responses during sentence reading: Visual input affects central processes. NeuroReport, 10(15), 3175-3178.
  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. ve Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439– 483.
  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. ve Osterhout, L. (1999). The neurocognition of syntactic processing. C.M. Brown ve P. Hagoort (Yay. haz.), The Neurocognition of Language içinde (ss. 273-316). Oxford University Press.
  • Hahne, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 11(2), 194-205.
  • Hahne, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2002). Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(3), 339-356.
  • Hartwigsen, G., Golombek, T. ve Obleser, J. (2015). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over left angular gyrus modulates the predictability gain in degraded speech comprehension. Cortex, 68, 100-110.
  • Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hahne, A., Schröger, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2011). Syntactic and auditory spatial processing in the human temporal cortex: An MEG study. Neuroimage, 57(2), 624-633.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 131-138.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92(1-2), 67-99.
  • Hickok, G. ve Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402.
  • Hofer, S. ve Frahm, J. (2006). Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited-comprehensive fiber tractography using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage, 32(3), 989-994.
  • Itzhak, I., Pauker, E., Drury, J.E., Baum, S.R. ve Steinhauer, K. (2010). Event-related potentials show online influence of lexical biases on prosodic processing. NeuroReport, 21(1), 8-13.
  • Kerkhofs, R., Vonk, W., Schriefers, H. ve Chwilla, D.J. (2007). Discourse, syntax, and prosody: The brain reveals an immediate interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1421-1434.
  • Kutas, M. ve Federmeier, K.D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470.
  • Kutas, M. ve Hillyard, S.A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–208.
  • MacSweeney, M., Capek, C.M., Campbell, R. ve Woll, B. (2008). The signing brain: The neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 432–440.
  • Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(20), 8362-8367.
  • McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L. ve Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703–704.
  • Meyer, M.L., Spunt, R.P., Berkman, E.T., Taylor, S.E. ve Lieberman, M.D. (2012).
  • Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 1883-1888.
  • Molinaro, N., Barber, H. A., Caffarra, S. ve Carreiras, M. (2015). On the left anterior negativity (LAN): The case of morphosyntactic agreement. Cortex, 66, 156-159.
  • Molinaro, N., Paz-Alonso, P.M., Duñabeitia, J.A. ve Carreiras, M. (2015). Combinatorial semantics strengthens angular-anterior temporal coupling. Cortex, 65, 113-127.
  • Neubert, F.X., Mars, R.B., Thomas, A.G., Sallet, J. ve Rushworth, M.F.S. (2014). Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron, 81(3), 700–713.
  • Obleser, J., Elbert, T., Lahiri, A. ve Eulitz, C. (2003). Cortical representation of vowels reflects acoustic dissimilarity determined by formant frequencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(3), 207-213.
  • Osterhout, L. ve Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(6), 785–806.
  • Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J. ve Swinney, D.A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786–803.
  • Petersson, K.M., Folia, V. ve Hagoort, P. (2012). What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax. Brain and Language, 120(2), 83-95.
  • Petersson, K.M., Forkstam, C. ve Ingvar, M. (2004). Artificial syntactic violations activate Broca's region. Cognitive Science, 28(3), 383-407.
  • Phillips, C., Pellathy, T., Marantz, A., Yellin, E., Wexler, K., Poeppel, D., McGinnis, M. ve Roberts, T. (2000). Auditory cortex accesses phonological categories: an MEG mismatch study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1038–55.
  • Rilling, J.K., Glasser, M.F., Preuss, T.M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X. ve Behrens, T.E.J. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 426–428.
  • Rogalsky, C. ve Hickok, G. (2011). The role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1664-1680.
  • Rossi, S., Gugler, M.F., Friederici, A.D. ve Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on syntactic second language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2030–2048.
  • Sammler, D., Kotz, S.A., Eckstein, K., Ott, D.V. ve Friederici, A.D. (2010). Prosody meets syntax: The role of the corpus callosum. Brain, 133(9), 2643-2655.
  • Santi, A. ve Grodzinsky, Y. (2010). fMRI adaptation dissociates syntactic complexity dimensions. Neuroimage, 51(4), 1285-1293.
  • Silva-Pereyra, J.F. ve Carreiras, M. (2007). An ERP study of agreement features in Spanish. Brain Research, 1185, 201-211.
  • Skeide, M.A. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 17, 323–332.
  • Skeide, M.A., Brauer, J. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). Brain functional and structural predictors of language performance. Cerebral Cortex, 26(5), 2127–2139.
  • Steinhauer, K. ve Drury, J.E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120(2), 135–162.
  • Steinhauer, K., Alter, K. ve Friederici, A.D. (1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 2(2), 191-196.
  • Tyler, L.K., Meredith, A.S., Randall, B., Wright, B., Marslen-Wilson, W.D. ve Stamatakis, E.A. (2010). Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: The modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2, 352–364.
  • Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A.C. ve Price, C.J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 550-560.
  • Wolff, S., Schlesewsky, M., Hirotani, M. ve Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2008). The neural mechanisms of word order processing revisited: Electrophysiological evidence from Japanese. Brain and Language, 107(2), 133-157.
  • Zaccarella, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2015). Merge in the human brain: A sub-region based functional investigation in the left pars opercularis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1818.
  • Zaccarella, E. ve Friederici, A.D. (2016). The neuroanatomical network of syntactic merge for language. MPI CBS, Research Report, 2014–2016 (s. 68). http://www.cbs.mpg.de/institute/research-reports.
APA Uzun I (2020). Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. , 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
Chicago Uzun Ipek Pinar Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. (2020): 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
MLA Uzun Ipek Pinar Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. , 2020, ss.323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
AMA Uzun I Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. . 2020; 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
Vancouver Uzun I Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. . 2020; 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
IEEE Uzun I "Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.." , ss.323 - 337, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
ISNAD Uzun, Ipek Pinar. "Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.". (2020), 323-337. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
APA Uzun I (2020). Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi, 31(2), 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
Chicago Uzun Ipek Pinar Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi 31, no.2 (2020): 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
MLA Uzun Ipek Pinar Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi, vol.31, no.2, 2020, ss.323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
AMA Uzun I Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi. 2020; 31(2): 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
Vancouver Uzun I Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.. Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi. 2020; 31(2): 323 - 337. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
IEEE Uzun I "Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.." Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi, 31, ss.323 - 337, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214
ISNAD Uzun, Ipek Pinar. "Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of auniquely human capacity. MIT Press.". Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi 31/2 (2020), 323-337. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/dad.841214